Wilkerson, Richard Catlett (1995). On the Tips our
Tongues: Clues from Dreaming Research to Enhancing our Control and Understanding
of Dream Recall . Electric Dreams 2(6), www.dreamgate.com/dream/ed-backissues/ed2-6.htm
On the Tips our Tongues: Clues from Dreaming Research to
Enhancing our Control and Understanding of Dream Recall
by Richard Catlett Wilkerson
The purpose of these essays is to explore how we can build our dreaming recall skills by an exploration of the last 50 years of dream research on recall. I want to mention that many informed dreamers see no need to increase the number or quality of dreams or build recall skills, and I just want to say that this essay is not placing a political or psychological value on recall, but on the self empowerment that comes from informed choices and options. All of the techniques offered might also be reversed to decrease dream recall and may thereby be of relevance to nightmare sufferers as well as dreamers who are seeking more dreams.
With the discovery of REM sleep and its connection with dreaming by
Aserinsky and Kleitman in the 1950's, empirical sleep research gave birth
to a new child, the study of dreams by respectable scientific researchers.
Now, 40 years later, what have we learned? As often happens, the researchers
are now beginning to confirm what the motivated dreamer has always known,
that the more interest you show towards your dreams, the more you have.
Still, since our hard earned tax dollars went into this research, maybe
we can get a little more from them than just the official go-ahead to keep
being interested.
Generally, dream recall research looks at issues of *content* or *process*.
The content theories include: salience (novelty, bizarreness, affectfulness,
or intensity), its opposite - dream disorganization (to chaotic to be remembered),
interference (example: body movements disrupt recall), disinterest in dreams,
and repression. The process theories are mostly memory- process oriented,
with the inclusion of arousal theories, state dependent learning and the
new neural net connectionist theories.
In this essay, we are going to look at the most commented on and least
clear reason for dream recall failure, the content theory of repression.
Due to the strong influence of psychodynamic dream theory, a major reason
postulated for the lack of dream recall is that dreams contain things we
just don't want to remember. This is content that the waking self just
can't handle or would cause too much distress if remembered. This dream
material is referred to as ego toxic. This follows from Freud's notion
that dreams are mediating desires that are pushing for expression and attention
and counter forces keeping those thoughts and impulses from disturbing
the sleeper. Most of the content is disguised before reaching consciousness,
but some gets kept away from the waking ego altogether. Hence the memory
loss. Or at least, this is the theory.
The clinicians feel that the notion is useful and the process easily observed
over time. New patients recall less dreams. As patients begin to show other
signs of less resistance to the material that the dream content is displaying,
more of this dream content shows up. The higher the repression, the lower
the content. However, these ideas are more anecdotal clinical observations
than tested research.
One approach to testing this is by using different personality measures.
Types that use repression as a defense ought to have less dreams to report,
right? The most popular method has been to test field-dependent types against
field-independent types. Generally, field- independent types are internally
cued; they eat and sleep when they feel tired or hungry and generally don't
repress or delay their desires if possible. Field-dependent individuals
are externally cued and eat and sleep (for example) according to an external
schedule and use something like repression/suppression on internal demands
and cues.
Yet the tests are mixed and not really convincing that it made any big
difference what type of field-dependency you had, though there is a suggestion
that field independent people recall dreams a little better. One interesting
notable group. These were field-dependent people who generally didn't remember
dreams if allowed to awaken in their usual way, but did remember many more
dreams when abruptly awakened from REM sleep. It seems the break in their
routine allowed for dream recall to increase. So, if you are a person who
lives by an external schedule and you want to remember more of your dreams,
you might try setting your alarm at random wake up times or have your schedule
interrupted by someone else. Also, if we were to act on this little evidence
of field-independence being of some help in recall, we might take it upon
ourselves to become more internally cued.
Finally, its not at all clear that field-dependence lack of dream recall
has anything to do with repression.
This same mixed results problem was found with personality tests using
the Convergent vs Divergent personalities. Are your better on multiple
choice tests that have one right answer (convergents are better on these)
or loose ended essay questions (divergent personalities)? There is some
indication that divergent personality types recall dream more frequently,
but slight indications only.
More Personality tests in relation to repression results:
Rorschach index of repressive styles:
In women, the repressors had less dreams.
But in men, just the *opposite* was found.
For those of you more interested in the gender issue, David Cohen did a
study on recall and sex role orientation (1973) where the issue of gender
was shifted to that of sex role orientation. I'm sure the masculine/agency
and feminine/communion connections would now be challenged, but it is probably
a positive alternative to explaining the above Rorschach results on gender
issues.
Repression-relevant questionnaire scales:
(repression-sensitization, anxiety, neuroticism, ego strength)
A couple, yes, many studies, no.
Is the problem that these studies didn't control for interest and salience,
(the two major factors for predicting dream recall), is personality in
general just not a very good predictor of dream recall, or what? Many researchers
now feel that it isn't. But what about repression in general? Can it still
be said to be playing a role if all the personality measures we use to
test for it don't give us any differences between individuals recalling
dreams?
There are a group of studies that use pre-sleep stress conditions. They
predicted that the pre-sleep stress would bring on repression and less
dream recall, which is exactly what happened. But it is often pointed out
that this may also be due to the distraction of attention upon awakening,
which also produces dream recall failure. Still, for those of us interested
in modulating recall, the choice of the evening's entertainment can be
experimented with as a personal factor.
A now famous study (Whitman, Kramer, Baldridge, 1963) had subjects report
dreams both in a laboratory setting and to therapists. The subjects often
withheld dreams from one while telling the other. Sometimes the therapist,
sometimes the lab recorder. Its not clear if they consciously withheld
or repressed the reports. But for us, we might consider that *who* we share
our dreams with may alter our recall.
Cultural repression is such a big issue that I'd like to unfold these
ideas in another essay. But I do want to note that I feel this is a large
factor in recall. I haven't seen any cross-cultural studies on this in
particular, but there are many anthropological studies that talk about
the common practice of daily dreamsharing of various culture's were the
parents do more than we who tell our children "Its just dream, dear,
go back to sleep."
Summarizing the repression study clues for recall.
Generally, it seems that repression plays a role, but just how it works
is not at all clear. Therapies and activities we take up that reduce our
repressive habits could increase our recall. Becoming more internally cued
and allowing for more divergent activities looks like a possibility. Reducing
anxiety before bedtime may decrease anxious images that could lead to a
repressed dream. Not always telling our dreams to the same person(s) or
internal person(s) might open up new channels of dreams that would be repressed
by our habits of dreamsharing to just one real or imaginary person. Included
in this may be re-visioning our inner mothers to tell us when we have a
dream something like "Oh, boy, that's a great dream, tell me more!"
And this may also lead us in another important direction. If we learn to
engage anxiety and imaginal fear a little more, we will be able to have
alternatives to just pushing odd and stressful things away. It is a useful
basic strategy to delay and set aside stressors, but not a long term solution
to everything. And so, one approach, like the child who is encouraged to
tell or draw a dream, is to do so ourselves. Different forms of storytelling
and dreamsharing may allow us to play with material that otherwise might
be too toxic and forgotten as useless.
I feel I have but barely touched upon the idea of dreams and repression
by going through a general summary of the empirical research. For one thing,
we have focused only on *amount* and not on *quality* of recall. But more
important than putting out a perfect summary I wanted to show that we could
as individuals use the available research to our advantage.
In the next essay I want to move through the other content oriented theories
of dream recall, focusing especially on salience and how dreams that are
seen as vivid, interesting and self-involving are the best content predictors
of recall.
A short Bibliography for Dream Recall Research & Cited articles:
Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of
eye motility, and concommitant phenomena, during sleep. _Science_, 118(3026),
273-274.
Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1955). Two types of ocular motility
occurring in sleep. _Journal of Applied Physiology_, 8(1), 1-10.
Cohen, David B. (1979). _Sleep and Dreaming: Origins, Nature and Functions_. New York: Pergamon Press.
--------. (1974a). Toward a theory of dream recall. _Psychological Bulletin_, 81(2), 138-154.
--------. (1974b). To sleep, perchance to recall a dream. _Psychology Today_, 7(12), May, 50-54.
--------. (1974c). Presleep mood and dream recall. _Journal of Abnormal Psychology_, 83(1), 45-51.
--------. (1973). Sex role orientation and dream recall. _ Journal of
Abnormal Psychology_, 82(2), 246-252.
Cohen, D. & Wolfe G. (1973). Dream recall and repression: Evidence
for an alternative hypothesis. _Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology_,
41(3), 349-355.
Freud, Sigmund. (1900/1953). The Interpretation of Dreams. Standard Edition,
4&5 London:Hogarth Press.
--------. (1965; first published 1900). _The Interpretation of Dreams._
James Strachey (Trans.). New York: Avon Books.
Goodenough, Donald R. (1991). Dream recall: History and current status
of the field. In : Ellman, Steven J. & Antrobus, John S. (Eds). (1991).
_The Mind in Sleep: Psychology and Psychophysiology._ 2nd editon. New York:
John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Goodenough, D., Witkin, H. A., Lewis, H. B. Koulack, D. Cohen, H.(1974).
Repression, interference and field dependence asfactors in dream forgetting.
_Journal of Abnormal Psychology_, 83(1), 32-44.
Gregory, Jill (1988) _Dream Tips_ Novato, CA: Novato Center for Dreams.
Moffitt, A., Kramer, M., Hoffmann, R. (Eds.). (1993). _The Function of
Dreaming._ NY: State University of New York Press.
Reed, Henry (1995). Encouraging dream recall. _Electric Dreams_ 2(6), electronic
page index.
Tonay, Veronica K. (1993). Personality correlates of dream recall: Who
remembers? _Dreaming,_ 3(1), 1-8.
Van De Castle, R. L. (1994). Our Dreaming Mind. New York: Ballantine Books.
Whitman, R., Kramer, M., & Baldridge, B. (1963). Which dream does the
patient tell? _Archives of General Psychiatry_, 8, 277-282.
Richard Wilkerson