Wilkerson, Richard Catlett (1995). Dreams of the
Blind. Electric Dreams 2(1), www.dreamgate.com/dream/ed-backissues/ed2-1.htm
Dreams of the Blind
By Richard Catlett Wilkerson
Though there has been little work done with dreams and the visually
impaired (Hunt, 1989, Rainville, 1994), the general belief and little evidence
seem to favor the notion that people dream as they live in waking life
and represent situations to themselves and others in pretty much the same
way in both wake and sleep. What confuses the issue is our habit or cognitive
style of verbal narratives that represent people and things in visual metaphors.
A quick look at the description of a dream by a blind individual may reveal
an elaborate array of visual imagery, while a closer examination of the
actual sensations of that blind dreamer reveal little or no direct visual
imagery.
Jastrow's early but major study of dreams of the blind (1900- Jastrow himself
was blind) is well worth reading and includes other studies of the time.
His research indicates that a majority of those who go blind before 5 to
7 years old will *not* have visual dreams. But though he attributes this
to brain development, it is not clear from his study if a lack of verbal
development of visual metaphors was considered.
In a special letter form Helen Keller, she relates what dreaming was like
before her teacher:
"My dreams have strangely changed during the past twelve years. Before
and after my teacher first came to me, they were devoid of sound, of thought
or emotion of any kind, except fear, and only came in the form of sensations.
I would often dream that I ran into a still, dark room, and that, while
I stood there, I felt something fall heavily without any noise, causing
the floor to shake up and down violently; and each time I woke up with
a jump. As I learned more and more about the objects around me, this strange
dream ceased to haunt me; but I was in a high state of excitement and received
impressions very easily. It is not strange then that I dreamed at the time
of a wolf, which seemed to rush towards me and put his cruel teeth deep
into my body! I could not speak (the fact was, I could only spell with
my fingers), and I tried to scream; but no sound escaped from my lips.
It is very likely that I had heard the story of Red Riding Hood, and was
deeply impressed by it. This dream, however, passed away in time, and I
began to dream of objects outside myself" (p. 353).
Later she relates, "I obtain information in a very curious manner,
which it is difficult to describe. My mind acts as a sort of mirror, in
which faces and landscapes are reflected, and thoughts, which throng unbidden
in my brain, describe the conversation and the events going on around me.
I remember a beautiful and striking illustration of the peculiar mode of
communication I have just mentioned. One night I dreamed that I was in
a lovely mansion, all built of leaves and flowers, My thoughts declared
the floor was of green twigs, and the ceiling of pink and white roses,
The wall were of roses, pinks, hyacinths, and many other flowers, loosely
arranged so as to make the whole structure wavy and graceful. Here and
there I saw an opening between the leaves, which admitted the purest air,
I learned that the flowers were imperishable, and with such a wonderful
discovery thrilling my spirit I awoke." (p. 354).
And yet, after such a imagistic account, she goes on to say, "I do
not think I have seen or heard more than once in my sleep. Then the sunlight
flashed suddenly on my eyes, and I was so dazzled I could not think or
distinguish anything, When I looked up some one spelled hastily to me,
'Why, you are looking back upon your babyhood!' ." (p. 354).
As Jastrow notices, "The dreams of seeing and hearing probably reflect
far more of conceptual interpretation and imaginative inference than of
true sensation; yet they are in part built up upon a sensory basis."
(p. 359). Notice the phrases " my thoughts declared," "my
mind acts as a sort of mirror," and "I was informed".
This notion that the narrative elaboration in dreams of the sighted and
blind remains constant even though specific visual imagery may vary has
been tested more recently in a study (Kerr, 1982) designed to control for
other cognitive abilities. The congenitally blind subjects without a history
of form vision were able to represent spatial relationships in dream experience
without either visual imagery or compensatory imagery in other modalities.
The congenitally blind subjects with minimal form vision saw in their dreams
only to the extent that they had been able to see in waking life. In neither
group did lack of visual imagery adversely affect the richness or narrative
continuity of dreaming. I'm including here a sample dream (about a cancer
clinic) taken from a congenitally blind subject in the Kerr study who has
light perception but no form detection abilities:
Subject(S): I was in a room that looked similar to my instant banker at
work, but it was a big machine with lots of buttons, like a car machine.
Experimenter(E): Like an instant banker machine?
S: Right, at {the bank}. And I don't know why I was there, but I guess
there was a screen and there were other buttons you could push, you could
look in and see how different cancer patients are doing.
E: Was this visual, could you see anything?
S: I couldn't, but I stood by the screen and I knew that *others* could
see what was going on through all the little panels...I guess I imagined
the board with the buttons. Maybe because I imagined in my mind, it was
not that I could really see them with my eyes, but I know what that board
looks like, and the only reason I know what it looks like is by touch,
and I could remember where the buttons were without touching them on the
boards... E: O.K. Where did the events in this experience seems to be taking
place? What were the settings?
S: It seemed to be a large room that was oblong in shape, and there seemed
to be an x-ray machine's work. I felt like it was in an office building
where I worked.
E: And you mentioned something before about the bank?
S: Un huh, it looked like the bank where I do my instant banking (E: O.K.),
except it was larger and more oblong.
E: And is that more like where you worked?
S: No, where I do work, the room is smaller, just large enough for that
little instant banker machine.
Kerr notes :
"This description of a novel setting illustrates that visual imagery
is not the only means by which spatial knowledge can be represented in
dreams. In fact, such knowledge need not depend on imaginal representation
in *any* sensory-specific modality. The subject was aware of the size and
shape of the room she was in, although she did not describe touching it
or waling around in it. She was aware of the observations panels and the
buttons on the machine without having to touch them. More generally, this
subject could create dream environments made up of elements from settings
familiar to her in waking life, but she was able to do so without representation
of specific sensations of either vision or touch." (p. 292).
Rather than saying that visually impaired individuals have limited dream
imagery, it would be a more useful and sophisticated position to say that
imagery is inspired and carried by visual components, but is not particularly
dependent upon visual elements. Rather, imagery is a cognitive conveyance,
a way of seeing rather than something seen. When H. Robert Blank, in his
article ( Dream analysis in the treatment of the blind, 1959) states that
the blind have no visual dreams and "This will surprise only those
who believe in a racial unconscious or the hereditary transmission of memories..."
(p. 190) he misses the point that imagery is not a visual perception, but
an psychological apperception.
The post-Jungian , James Hillman, in _The Dream and the Underworld_ (1973/1979)
further unfolds how extensive this visual bias is in what he feels is our
societies greatest cause of psychological illness, our inability to be
imaginal and metaphorical, our continual insistence on literalness (for
example, suicide is seen as the persons confusing the imaginal need for
drastic rebirth with the literal act of self destruction.
Jung is now famous for his saying the same thing about drinking - that
the person mistakes the metaphorical need for spiritual contact with 'the
spirit in the bottle'). It is interesting that one of the great interpreters
of dreams, the ancient Greek Tiresias, was blind. Perhaps in our listening
to the dreams of the visually impaired, we may, like the those who encountered
Tiresias, come to see our own blindness.
Annotated Bibliography
Adelson, Edward T.(Ed.). (1963). Dream analysis in
the treatment of the blind. In _Dreams in Contemporary
Psychoanalysis_. ( pp. 188-211) New York: The Society of
Medical Psychoanalysts. (Symposium on
dreams, NY 1959). [Some important considerations for
clinical work and psychodynamic insights about the issues
that will arise around blindness as castration and the
identifications of the victim with the castrating father
and social majority that may lead to self persecution.]
Hillman, James (1979). _Dreams and the Underworld_.
New York: Harper and Row, Publishers, Inc.
--------. (1973). The dream and the underworld._
Eranos_, 42: 237-31. [Two versions of the same writing.
Many feel put off by the references, the obscurity, the
digressions, the hard questions put to dream
interpretation, psychology and society in general. It's my
favorite dream book.]
Hunt, Harry. (1989). _The Multiplicity of Dreams:
Memory, Imagination and Consciousness_. New Haven: Yale
University Press. [Simply the finest readable summary of
research into cognitive studies on dreaming.]
Kerr, Nancy H., Foulkes, D., & Schmidt, M. (1982).
The structure of laboratory dream reports in blind and
sighted subjects. _The Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease_, 170:(5?), 286-294. [ A good cognitive dream
research study. A quote from the abstract " Overall, the
results are consistent with the view that the dream is a
constructive cognitive process, rather than a reproductive
perceptual one, and with the view that the integrative
cognitive systems responsible for both the momentary and
the sequential organization of the dream do not depend on
the presence either of contemporaneous visual-perceptual
experience or of well developed visual cognitive codes." p.
287]
Jastrow, J. (1900). The dreams of the blind. In _
Fact and Fable in Psychology_. (pp. 337-370). Boston:
Houghton Mifflin Co. [Worth reading just for Helen
Keller's report, but also good summary of research at the
time. Jastrow was himself blind.]
Rainville, Raymond E. (1994). The role of dreams in
the rehabilitation of the adventitiously blind.
_Dreaming_, 4:(3), 155-164. [Very interesting and useful
information for clinical work and the vital role that
dreams play in the life adjustment of the newly blind. Also
a great bibliography on dreams and the blind and an
interesting notation of which of
the listed authors were themselves blind.]
Richard Wilkerson (1995)